As social beings, we’re very good at letting others know
when we’ve accidentally stopped being social. When we’ve missed something
important in a conversion, we interrupt and ask: “Pardon?” “Excuse me?”
“Sorry?” “What?” “Huh?” Sometimes furrowing
the eyebrows a bit can suffice to communicate a sense of confusion.
We need ways to communicate when we’re lost because – let’s
face it – we get lost often. We could be looking a fellow conversationalist
directly in the eyes, convincing ourselves that we’re taking it all in as we
listen to them clearly enunciate each syllable, when in reality every word is
slipping right over our heads. “Come again?” we bid.
These lapses in attention could be driven by distracting
thoughts or events in our environment, but are such diversions necessary for a
conversation space-out to occur? A new study in press in NeuroImage suggests the possibility that our brains are constantly
fluctuating in and out of particular states of attention. In certain brain
states, we’re prepared to take in information and learn about it, whereas in
other brain states, new information will likely elude us.
Study participants were scanned with functional MRI as they
viewed 250 photographs of indoor and outdoor scenes, presented one at a time.
The subjects were told that after the brain scan, they would be taking a test
that would assess which scenes they recognize from the scan. In that test, the
subjects were presented with images that either had or had not appeared during
the scan, and for each image the subjects rated how confident they were that
they had previously seen the indoor/outdoor scene. Out of the 250 images, the
participants only correctly remembered some of them.
When the researchers analyzed the subjects’ brain
activations, it was found that lower levels of activation in the
parahippocampal place area (PPA, a brain region that is known to respond to
visual scenes) occurring 2 seconds before presentation of an image predicted
that an image would subsequently be correctly remembered in the post-scan test.
However, when PPA activation was a bit higher during the 2 second period before
an image was presented, the subject was likely to forget that image.
This is pretty neat. It shows that fluctuations in brain
activity before we are presented with
information determine whether we’ll pay attention to and remember that
information. These sorts of fluctuations may also partially explain why we
sometimes tune out of conversations.
But the neater part of this study was the follow-up. Based
on the first experiment, the researchers called downward fluctuations in PPA
activity a “good” brain state (ready to learn and remember a scene image) and
upward fluctuations in PPA activity a “bad” brain state (likely not ready to
process the image). The researchers used a technology known as “real-time fMRI”
to monitor “good” or “bad” fluctuations in brain state as they were happening.
When either a good or bad brain state occurred, a scene image was presented. As
expected, when good brain states triggered an image, 2 hours later the image
was more likely to be remembered than when a bad brain state had initially triggered
an image.
This real-time fMRI study is particularly interesting
because it demonstrates that fMRI can do more than just identify the neural
correlates of human behaviours or perceptions. Rather than using stimuli to
elicit brain activation, this study used brain activation to drive stimulus
presentation and cause a certain
behaviour (good or bad learning). Using brain activity to elicit stimuli, we can
get closer to the question of a causal
relationship that can otherwise only be addressed by inducing brain damage or
using brain stimulation/other techniques to interrupt neural activity.
This study also might have implications for education programs
that require “optimized” brain states for efficient learning to occur. And of
course, there may be implications for optimizing everyday conversations. Next
time you accidentally drift out of a conversation, consider adding the phrase “pardon
my brain” to your repertoire of strategies for communicating that you’re not
all there at the moment.
Reference: